Autism and communication
Research suggests autistic people may have different communication styles and preferences to non-autistic people. Communication differences must be present for an autism diagnosis, but these can vary widely between autistic people.
What do we mean by communication?
Communication includes a wide range of skills and preferences.
Social communication and interaction
‘Social communication’ and ‘social interaction’ are often used interchangeably. This includes the use and understanding of spoken (verbal) and non-spoken (non-verbal) communication, such as conversation and body language. It also includes social ‘exchanges’, such as sharing and taking turns.
Social communication and interaction skills are used to start, build and maintain friendships and relationships with other people, including in your personal life, in education and at work.
Social communication and interaction differences are part of the criteria for an autism diagnosis, but these differences can vary widely between autistic people.
Language
Language is a system of sounds and symbols used in communication.
Although language differences are not part of the criteria for an autism diagnosis, language is a significant part of social communication and interaction, and research suggests that some autistic people may have differences in how they understand and/or use language. This can vary a lot between autistic people.
Autistic communication differences
Social communication and social interaction differences (compared to non-autistic people) are a ‘core characteristic’ of autism. For a diagnosis of autism to be made, these differences must:
- have been present since birth
- have a clinically significant impact on the person’s day to day life.
However, the presence and noticeability of these characteristics can vary a lot between autistic people.
Evidence suggests that, in comparison to non-autistic people, autistic people might:
- prefer to work, play or spend time alone
- use more:
- direct language, or less unnecessary or ambiguous language (sometimes interpreted as being ‘blunt’)
- formal, precise and technical speech
- pauses without filling them with ‘um’ or ‘ah’ sounds
- repeated words and phrases (echolalia)
- unique invented words
- find it more challenging to:
- recognise or read body language or facial expressions
- choose a tone of voice, volume or speed of speech
- judge pauses and turn-taking, or to be interrupted during a conversation
- understand words when they are not used literally, for example in metaphors, idioms, jokes, irony and sarcasm
- have a different pattern of language development compared to non-autistic people, which may include losing language skills (regression) and a later catch-up in childhood
- prefer not to make eye-contact, or find it uncomfortable
- use fewer gestures (generally in childhood).
Visit our page Signs that a child or adult may be autistic for examples relating to social communication and social interaction differences, and other autism characteristics.
Speech differences
Autistic children, like non-autistic children, develop speech at different rates. Some have typical speech development, and some show advanced language use; others have delayed speech development with a later catch up.
Some autistic people do not develop speech or use very limited speech (a few words but not sentences) throughout their lives. This is usually when they have a co-occurring learning disability.
Some autistic people who often or usually use speech can experience intermittent speech loss or an unreliable ability to speak (their ability to speak comes and goes). This is often related to their situation; for example, they may find it extremely difficult or be unable to speak when they are overwhelmed, stressed or experiencing autistic burnout.
People who are not able to speak, either temporarily or in the long term, are sometimes described as being ‘non-verbal’. However, some people with lived experience do not prefer this term, because it:
- may not be an accurate description of their experience (either because they can sometimes speak, or because they can ‘verbalise’, sometimes speaking words, sometimes using sounds not words)
- can be misinterpreted to mean that they can’t communicate, which is wrong (there are other ways of communicating that don’t involve speaking words).
Describing a person’s individual use of speech is generally preferred to blanket labels.
Another preferable and commonly used phrase is ‘people who speak few or no words’. While this may be appropriate for people who do not use speech (or who use very limited speech), it is not always relevant for people whose ability to use speech can change over time or depending on the situation (for example when they are overwhelmed.)
People who do not use speech, or who sometimes cannot use speech, may communicate in other ways, for example by using:
- writing
- sign language, such as British Sign Language (BSL)
- Augmentative and Alternative Communication (AAC), such as picture-based systems or high-tech communication devices
- vocalisations (making sounds using their vocal cords but not words)
- gestures (pointing, eye gaze).
Read about the experiences of autistic people and parents on communication differences, including speech differences, in the section below, Insight from autistic people and parents.
Different social skills
Research into autistic communication
Research into autistic communication is unfortunately often framed as the study of perceived ‘deficits’ compared to non-autistic people. This ‘deficits-based’ approach can suggest that non-autistic communication styles and preferences are ‘correct’ or ‘superior’, and that autistic people ‘lack’ communication skills in comparison.
Developments in our understanding of autism suggest that, rather than lacking skills, autistic people might have different skills or communication preferences. (This relates to the ‘double empathy problem', explained below.)
Some research suggests autistic people may share information more effectively and have greater rapport, flow and empathy with other autistic people than they do with non-autistic people.
There isn’t enough research into autistic communication differences, including research about autistic people with advanced language skills and autistic people with learning disabilities.
What does this mean for support?
Traditionally, support strategies and therapies have expected autistic people to do all the work to bridge the gap in communication styles and preferences between them and non-autistic people.
These interventions have focused on training autistic people to act more like non-autistic people, often by ‘masking’ their autistic characteristics, communication styles and preferences. But masking can be exhausting and cause considerable stress and anxiety for autistic people, which can lead to mental health problems.
For information about best practice, see the section Communication support and strategies below.
The double empathy problem
The ‘double empathy problem’ is a theory from the autistic autism researcher Dr Damian Milton. It tries to explain why non-autistic people can experience autistic people as lacking in communication skills, including empathy. The theory states that there is a two-way problem, and that autistic people often experience non-autistic people as lacking in these skills too.
The theory suggests that this is because the two groups of people have different experiences and their different communication styles and preferences can lead to difficulties in relating to each other. Neither is ‘correct’, nor ‘superior’ to the other, they are simply different, and effort must be made on both sides to bridge the gap.
Evidence from lived experience (from autistic people) and some research suggests the double empathy problem theory may explain the challenges of and differences in communication between autistic and non-autistic people.
“Simply put, the theory of the double empathy problem suggests that when people with very different experiences of the world interact with one another, they will struggle to empathise with each other. This is likely to be exacerbated through differences in language use and comprehension”
– Dr Damian Milton, The double empathy problem
The impact of communication differences
Evidence suggests that autistic people’s communication differences, and the way that society fails to accept and accommodate these differences, can lead to:
- judgement and rejection by peers (including bullying)
- difficulty making and maintaining friendships
- tiredness and fatigue after social interaction
- anxiety about social situations
- feelings of frustration, embarrassment, self-consciousness and low self-esteem
- increased risk of feeling isolated and lonely
- challenges engaging in education and work
- mental health problems (anxiety and depression)
- masking – forcing yourself to act like non-autistic people, in line with social ‘norms’, which negatively impacts autistic people.
Communication support and strategies
Support strategies
The aim of any communication support or intervention should be to reduce barriers and enhance a person’s ability to communicate and participate equally in society. Planned outcomes should be based on the views and goals of the person, whether they are a child, young person or adult.
When considering a therapy or intervention, it is important that:
- professionals have good autism knowledge, and the therapy should be adapted to meet the person’s specific needs
- the autistic person is central to all decisions
- potential negative effects are considered
- impact should be carefully monitored and regularly reviewed.
Interventions might include:
- speech and language therapy
- play-based social learning programmes which should increase parent/carer/teacher/peer’s understanding of and sensitivity to the individual’s style of communication
- talking therapies in which autistic people learn strategies to cope with anxiety during social interactions
- Intensive Interaction – an approach to develop the communication of people who have not developed speech and people with learning disabilities
- Alternative and Augmentative Communication (AAC) – for people who cannot use speech, or use very limited speech, either temporarily or in the long term.
Please be aware that Facilitated communication (FC) is not recommended as an intervention for autistic people.
Self-managed strategies
Some autistic people find strategies such as these helpful:
- finding out what to expect before social situations and preparing
- enlisting a social ally (a person who can provide support during/after social situations)
- being selective about social events (and not being afraid to say ‘no’)
- taking time out after social situations, to regulate anxiety and recharge
- having a plan if social situations become overwhelming (e.g. using ear defenders and having access to a quiet space)
- socialising through online platforms
- making connections with people who have similar interests and/or other autistic people.
What non-autistic people can do
Some autistic people have said they find it useful when non-autistic people:
- show respect and patience for their preferences
- break spoken instructions down and allow for extra processing time
- accept all forms of communication as valid (not just speech)
- recognise that behaviour is a means of communication.
How is communication assessed in an autism assessment?
Autism assessments can vary, but NICE guidelines recommend that a specialist team of professionals should be involved, including:
- a Speech and Language Therapist (SALT)
- paediatricians and/or psychiatrists
- clinical and/or educational psychologist.
For more detailed information, visit our page Professionals involved in an autism assessment.
To better understand a person’s profile and ensure the accuracy of autism diagnosis, NICE guidelines also recommend the specialist team collect information about social communication skills:
- from a child’s school/s and nursery/nurseries
- from an adult’s workplace/s or other environments that involve social activity (with their consent).
Further assessment of language and social communication
Following an autism diagnosis, a more detailed assessment of language and social communication can be sought from a Speech and Language Therapist (SALT) at any point in a person’s life. This is most likely to happen if communication barriers are impacting a child or young person’s access to education. Unlike an autism assessment, this can highlight specific areas of communication that need support.
Cultural differences
Tools used to assess autism, such as the ADOS-2, are based on western European and north American cultural norms. It may be more difficult to both accurately recognise and clinically assess differences in social communication and interaction in people from cultures with other norms using these tools.
When carrying out assessments, professionals should be aware that an individual’s social habits (such as the use of eye contact) may be culturally specific. The language of assessment (and treatment) should also be carefully considered for people from bilingual households.
Gender differences
Research suggests that autistic women and girls can differ from autistic men and boys in their social communication. Compared with autistic men and boys, autistic women and girls may:
- have social communication and social interaction skills that are more like non-autistic people
- express emotion more similarly to non-autistic people
- be more likely to mask their social differences and natural style of speech and so appear more like non-autistic people.
-
Autistic people
“All people on the spectrum have a valid and functional ways of communicating ... – I find that I write more emotively than I verbally express and when I was a child who didn’t speak with my mouth for 5 years I had many words within me. … The way in which I connected with people was through ‘sounds’, ‘movements’ and ‘noises’ which were functionally right for me; it was my own language before interpretive language, I still live in this world and treat interpretive language as acquired but not my ‘native’ language.”
– Paul Isaacs, All communication is valid – none are inferior“The best analogy I can give is to describe my speech as a heavy spinning flywheel. The speed the flywheel is spinning represents the amount of speech I have. Sometimes the flywheel stops spinning and it’s very hard to get spinning again. Other times it spins really fast and I find it hard to control my speech. Losing my speech is normally triggered by my environment or stress levels. The quickest way to recover my speech is to fix the underlying issue.”
– Jamie + Lion, My speech“If I am on sensory overload due to my children being noisy and running and jumping on me, my verbal ability can almost completely shut down. I want to say something meaningful rather than yell, but the words will not come out.”
– Onelonedandelion, Verbal and written communication: the disconnect“I was in speech therapy until I was eleven. ... I have to wonder, were the years spent training me to talk ‘better’ worth it? … With AAC it’s like a giant weight I hadn’t fully realised was there has been lifted. For example, in sign language I find myself outgoing and energised by social interaction, the complete opposite of my other experiences socialising.”
– endever*, We deserve access to AAC!“It is unfortunately very common for professionals to disbelieve our reports of having pain or disabilities in other environments, when we present in clinical and educational settings as calm, clear communicators. They assume our day to day skills will be consistent, but they assume wrongly. What they are missing is our mask.”
– Trauma Geek, Lost in translation: the social language theory of neurodivergence“I’m very good at talking, I know several other autistic people who also are; performers, magicians, lawyers amongst them. The thought of speaking in public, whilst it may bring on a certain level of anxiety, it concerns us a lot less than many other people…. The key difference, for me is that this is not a conversation. … Conversation is, for me, a complex minefield of social protocol, implication and sub-context.”
– Paul, The problems with talking“One of my worst nightmares is being asked something that I am not prepared for, or for a conversation to go a way that I haven’t rehearsed in my head. I will spend hours going over all the possible permutations of a conversation, just in case.”
– Rosemarie, What the world doesn't see“If I’m talking about something that excites me (read: anything special interest related), my voice will gradually gain volume, soon becoming way too loud for the situation. Somehow I don’t notice this happening until it’s painfully obvious by the look on the other person’s face. Or they just outright tell me to tone it down.”
– Cynthia Kim, Prosody: loud voice, fast voice, soft voice, flat voice“When someone is talking to me, their words arrive at my ears in a vague, jumbled up way. It takes me a while to unravel the words, and then turn them into something tangible that I can get some meaning from. ... It’s a game of verbal ‘catchup’ and I gradually find myself falling further and further behind in the race to process the conversation. I often end up with only the vaguest idea of what the conversation is about.”
– Steve, Autism: auditory dyslexiaParents
“Speech is actually not the most important thing after all: communication is! I was so caught up with panic at the fact my toddler didn’t say any words that I hadn’t noticed how he was communicating! His eyes, his body language, his facial expressions, his noises all communicated in ways that words couldn’t.”
– Miriam Gwynne, Three things I have learnt about children’s speech from my non verbal son“Nonverbal communication is a pretty amazing thing really. My son Cooper communicates mostly with sounds: Think grunts and squeals and gasps. He points a lot. And brings things to us and brings us to things. … He has some words. … You might not understand him, but me, I know every word because we spent hours, days, weeks, months and years practicing. … Every sound is a gift. Because I saw the mountains he had to climb to get them.”
– Kate Swenson, Finding Cooper’s voice“What I call intensive interaction is basically communicating with him by following his lead. We all make noises at babies and they react so of course we can do it with older kids too. If he makes noises I copy them, or arm movements, or pretty much anything. We have ... lots of eye contact and laughs and giggles, sometimes he tells me a story making all sorts of amazing sounds, or a game of peekaboo or clicking noises. The joy it brings both of us is just the best.”
– Nick, The importance of communication – a dad’s view
-
“I need to control not just how I respond, but also my facial features and body language, when someone says something that I don't agree with. At times, I am literally having to fight my natural reflexive reactions as I find it difficult to hide what I am thinking. It is exhausting.”
– Helen, Unmasked #TakeTheMaskOff“A little glimpse of what … masking has been like for me with eye contact … it's something that I've done so much that I don't even think about it and it's totally comfortable in so many situations, but then when I start to lose it I'll just feel like it's slipping, slipping … and I'll feel my face just falling apart and I can't get it back and then it's all over. Like, I'm so awkward I need to get out of the situation and decompress.”
– Cammie, Autism and eye contact“Before I learned I was autistic, I didn't look for information in people's faces, or their body language, to see how they were feeling or even to see if they were interested in things I was saying. It just never really occurred to me that there was information there. … Body language is like this secret decoder you can use to figure out what other people are feeling. That's pretty powerful because I didn't have these tools for a large portion of my life.”
– Lyric Rivera, I'm autistic and this is why I find people overwhelming“Motion is essential to our emotional processing and helps us express our feelings. We stim: tap, dance, flap, bounce, spin, rock, and wiggle, in order to stay in touch with our Inner Selves and our intense emotional world. When we are not allowed to do these things, we shut down on the inside and outside.”
– Trauma Geek, Lost in translation: the social language theory of neurodivergence
-
“Autistic/neurodivergent people are already working so hard, in ways that you often can’t see… just participating in a conversation is an incredible amount of work. Allistic/neurotypical folks taking on responsibility for a more balanced give-and-take and allowing for some grace around all of this... may not even be half of the load.”
– Adam, “That’s so rude!” Is it, though?“I find it funny when people write about autism as a primarily social disability. … Autistic people get along great with other autistic people. … The funnier thing though, is that the feeling is mutual. Autistic people perceive non-autistic people as ‘lacking in social skills’... Autistic people don’t lack social skills. Autistic people have different social skills.”
– Quincy, Different social skills, not a lack thereof“Neurodivergent people have been stereotyped as having difficulty with understanding other people’s thought processes, a skill called mindreading. However, neurotypicals also experience mindblindness when communicating with neurodivergent people. Neurotypicals cannot intuitively understand our minds.”
– Trauma Geek, Lost in translation: the social language theory of neurodivergence
-
“I’ve always had lots of friends, because I am a social person, but in the past I think I was making a lot of compromises. From childhood kind of learning that the only way I could get friends was if I shaped myself to be who I thought they wanted me to be, if I didn’t come across as too needy, if I didn’t come across as too weird, until ultimately you’re basically not even being yourself, so it’s not even a real friendship, and it’s only really since getting my diagnosis that that’s changed.”
– Purple Ella, Friends when you're autistic (a chat with my neurodivergent friends)“For me, the line between friend and colleague is so unclear that I never know what I'm doing. I have no idea of what is and isn't appropriate or when someone is being nice to me because they like me – and not just for their own reasons.”
– Gavin, Friendship isn't so obvious when you're on the spectrum“I’ve heard people say (even relatives of mine) that those with autism don’t like talking to people and they don’t want to have friends. They just like being alone. This for one is incorrect. There are a lot of autistic people who struggle to socialise and have very few friends, but that doesn’t mean that they don’t want friends.”
– Alex, Are people with autism unsociable?“When I was in my teens up to even my very early 20s, I had a tendency to try too hard to act cool but fail miserably at it. When I was 13 I became so aware of just how different I was from other kids. Other teenagers were silly and did daft things. I wasn’t like this at all. I tried to act like them but I could never quite get it right…. I came to the realisation when I was about 14/15 that pretending to be something I’m not is a terrible idea.”
– Alex, Struggling to act ‘cool’ and silly as a teen with autism“Autistic social motivation is deeply rooted in the desire to share knowledge and in the desire to learn, and this has big implications for the protocols that are used in autistic communication. In contrast, the societies we grow up in and live in value abstract social status symbols more than developing a shared understanding, and this leads to the communication challenges that define our social experiences.”
– Jorn, What can be misunderstood will be misunderstood
-
“One of the most infuriatingly incorrect stereotypes about autistic people is that we are ‘robots’ that don’t feel anything. … When I say I love being autistic it is predominantly because of my hyper-empathy. To live life so connected and so emotionally responsive. To watch a butterfly and have to physically contain myself from showing the happiness it can bring me. To feel so strongly about doing what I can to bring kindness to others and help people.”
– Rosie Weldon, Autism and emotions“I’m sorry I’m so friendly but if you don’t like it, I leave you alone. There doesn’t seem to be any in between that I can find. That’s how stark my empathy is. Whether it’s all or whether it’s nothing, it’s always too much. It doesn’t matter that I tried to be nice or act polite. When other people do it, they accept the fact that they’re not trying to hurt their feelings. It’s just not the case for me. There is no way for me to mask or act normal. So I’ll just be me.”
– Matthew Weatherford, What it's like having hyperempathy as an autistic person“It's hard to really convey emotions, so one way that I get people to do it is using emojis on your… smartphone … because you get a text message and somebody has a crying face, you know instantly this person's upset, there's no ambiguity, there's no having to guess if this person's okay, no beating around the bush.”
– Dan, This is how you communicate with autistic people (5 tips)“To me, emotions are like storms at sea. They are mysterious and unpredictable. I feel like I can go from zero to furious in 0.4 seconds because I am unable to see all the intermediate shades of emotion along the route to furious, so when I finally arrive at that destination, it feels to me as if it came out of nowhere. ... Meditation helps a lot. My role model is Mr. Spock from Star Trek. Vulcans have emotions but practice meditation and other rituals from an early age to learn to control their emotions rather than allowing their emotions to control them.”
– Maxfield, Alexithymia: I don’t know how I feel
-
- Autism Services Directory: for services and support
- Branches: offering support, information and social activities for autistic adults, children and their families in their local areas
- Community: our online community is a place for autistic people and their families to meet like-minded people and share their experiences
- Diagnostic and Assessment Service: our national specialist service for children, young people and adults led by the Lorna Wing Centre
- Autism Know How: our autism training and best practice services.
-
- Autism, NHS Inform (Scotland)
- NICE guidelines, Diagnostic assessment by an autism team
- RCSLT, Guidance for speech and language therapists
- Factsheet: Intensive interaction, British Institute of Learning Disabilities
-
Research
Author: Centelles, L. et al (2013)
Research: From Action to Interaction: Exploring the Contribution of Body Motion Cues to Social Understanding in Typical Development and in Autism Spectrum Disorders
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 43, pp. 1140-1150Author: Nackaerts, E. et al (2012)
Research: Recognizing Biological Motion and Emotions from Point-Light Displays in Autism Spectrum Disorders
Source: PLoS ONE. Vol. 7, pp. 1-12Author: Mishra, A. et al (2021)
Research: Gesture Production in Toddlers with Autism Spectrum Disorder
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 5, pp. 1658-1667Author: Özçalışkan, Ş. et al (2016)
Research: Early deictic but not other gestures predict later vocabulary in both typical development and autism
Source: Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice. Vol. 20, pp. 754-763Author: Dimitrova, N. et al (2017)
Research: Do Verbal Children with Autism Comprehend Gesture as Readily as Typically Developing Children?
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 47, pp. 3267-3280Author: Ramos-Cabo, S. et al (2019)
Research: Gesture and Language Trajectories in Early Development: An Overview From the Autism Spectrum Disorder Perspective
Source: Frontiers in Psychology. Vol. 10, pp. 1-9Author: Talbott, M. et al (2020)
Research: The Developmental Sequence and Relations Between Gesture and Spoken Language in Toddlers With Autism Spectrum Disorder
Source: Child Development. Vol. 91, pp. 743-753Author: Medeiros, K. and Winsler, A. (2014)
Research: Parent–Child Gesture Use During Problem Solving in Autistic Spectrum Disorder
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 44, pp. 1946-1958Author: Blume, J. et al (2020)
Research: Language Growth in Young Children with Autism: Interactions Between Language Production and Social Communication
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 51, pp. 644-665Author: Pickard, K. and Ingersoll, B. (2015)
Research: Brief Report: High and Low Level Initiations of Joint Attention, and Response to Joint Attention: Differential Relationships with Language and Imitation
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 45, pp. 262-268Author: Hurwitz, S. and Watson, L. (2016)
Research: Joint attention revisited: Finding strengths among children with autism
Source: Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice. Vol. 20, pp. 538-550Author: Caruana, N. et al (2018)
Research: Joint attention difficulties in autistic adults: An interactive eye-tracking study
Source: Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice. Vol. 22, pp. 502-512Author: Wong, C. and Kasari, C. (2012)
Research: Play and Joint Attention of Children with Autism in the Preschool Special Education Classroom
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 42, pp. 2152-2161Author: Lee, K. and Schertz, H. (2020)
Research: Brief Report: Analysis of the Relationship Between Turn Taking and Joint Attention for Toddlers with Autism
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 50, pp. 2633-2640Author: Damianidou, D. et al (2020)
Research: The Use of Robots in Social Communications and Interactions for Individuals with ASD: a Systematic Review
Source: Advances in Neurodevelopmental Disorders. Vol. 4, pp. 357-388Author: Jones, R. et al (2017)
Research: Increased Eye Contact During Conversation Compared to Play in Children With Autism
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 47, pp. 607-614Author: Madipakkam, A. et al (2017)
Research: Unconscious avoidance of eye contact in autism spectrum disorder
Source: Scientific Reports. Vol. 7, pp. 1-6Author: Corbett, B. et al (2014)
Research: Examining the relationship between face processing and social interaction behavior in children with and without autism spectrum disorder
Source: Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders. Vol. 6, pp. 1-11Author: Trevisan, D. et al (2018)
Research: Facial Expression Production in Autism: A Meta‐Analysis
Source: Autism Research. Vol. 11, pp. 1586-1601Author: Eack, S. et al (2015)
Research: Misinterpretation of facial expressions of emotion in verbal adults with autism spectrum disorder
Source: Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice. Vol. 19, pp. 308-315Author: Grossman, R. and Tager-Flusberg, H. (2012)
Research: “Who Said That?” Matching of Low- and High-Intensity Emotional Prosody to Facial Expressions by Adolescents with ASD
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 42, pp. 2546-2557Author: Doi, H. et al (2013)
Research: Recognition of Facial Expressions and Prosodic Cues with Graded Emotional Intensities in Adults with Asperger Syndrome
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 43, pp. 2099-2113Author: Taylor, L. et al (2015)
Research: Evidence for shared deficits in identifying emotions from faces and from voices in autism spectrum disorders and specific language impairment
Source: International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. Vol. 50, pp. 452-466Author: Hubbard, D. et al (2017)
Research: Production and perception of emotional prosody by adults with autism spectrum disorder
Source: Autism Research. Vol. 10, pp. 1991-2001Author: Scheerer, N. et al (2020)
Research: Affective Prosody Perception and the Relation to Social Competence in Autistic and Typically Developing Children
Source: Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. Vol. 48, pp. 1-11Author: Zhang, M. et al (2021)
Research: Recognition of affective prosody in autism spectrum conditions: A systematic review and meta-analysis
Source: Autism. Volume OnlineFirst, pp. 1-16Author: Oerlemans, A. et al (2014)
Research: Recognition of facial emotion and affective prosody in children with ASD (+ADHD) and their unaffected siblings
Source: European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Vol. 23, pp. 257-271Author: Bottema-Beutel, K. et al (2014)
Research: The Role of Supported Joint Engagement and Parent Utterances in Language and Social Communication Development in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 44, pp. 2162-2174Author: Lai, S-S. et al (2020)
Research: Identifying the Cognitive Correlates of Reciprocity in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 50, pp. 2007-2018Author: Backer van Ommeren, T. et al (2015)
Research: Reliability and Validity of the Interactive Drawing Test: A Measure of Reciprocity for Children and Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 45, pp.Author: Backer van Ommeren, T. et al (2012)
Research: Measuring Reciprocity in High Functioning Children and Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 42, pp. 1001-1010Author: Lai, S-S. et al (2020)
Research: Identifying the Cognitive Correlates of Reciprocity in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 50, pp. 2007-2018Author: Hartley, C. and Fisher, S. (2018)
Research: Do Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Share Fairly and Reciprocally?
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 48, pp. 2718-2726Author: Backer van Ommeren, T, et al (2017)
Research: Sex differences in the reciprocal behaviour of children with autism
Source: Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice. Vol. 21, pp. 795-803Author: Matthewman, H. et al (2021)
Research: Comparing Frequency of Listener Responses Between Adolescents with and Without ASD During Conversation
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. OnlineFirst, pp. 1-12Author: Bauminger-Zviely, N. et al (2017)
Research: Speech Acts During Friends’ and Non-friends’ Spontaneous Conversations in Preschool Dyads with High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder versus Typical Development
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 47, pp. 1380-1390Author: Song, Y. et al (2019)
Research: Empathy Impairment in Individuals With Autism Spectrum Conditions From a Multidimensional Perspective: A Meta-Analysis
Source: Frontiers in Psychology. Vol. 10, pp. 1-27Author: Montgomery, C. et al (2016)
Research: Do Adults with High Functioning Autism or Asperger Syndrome Differ in Empathy and Emotion Recognition?
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 46, pp. 1931-1940Author: Deschamps, P. et al (2014)
Research: Empathy and Empathy Induced Prosocial Behavior in 6- and 7-Year-Olds with Autism Spectrum Disorder
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 44, pp. 1749-1758Author: Trimmer, E. et al (2017)
Research: Not knowing what I feel: Emotional empathy in autism spectrum disorders
Source: Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice. Vol. 21, pp. 450-457Author: Stevanovic, M. et al (2019)
Research: Physiological responses to affiliation during conversation: Comparing neurotypical males and males with Asperger syndrome
Source: PLoS ONE. Vol. 14, pp. 1-19Author: Crompton, C. et al (2020)
Research: Autistic peer-to-peer information transfer is highly effective
Source: Autism. Vol. 24, pp. 1704-1712Author: Milton, D. (2012)
Research: On the ontological status of autism: the ‘double empathy problem’
Source: Disability and Society. Vol. 27, pp. 883-887.Author: Komeda, H. et al (2019)
Research: Do Individuals With Autism Spectrum Disorders Help Other People With Autism Spectrum Disorders? An Investigation of Empathy and Helping Motivation in Adults With Autism Spectrum Disorder
Source: Frontiers in Psychiatry. Vol. 10, pp. 1-9Author: Griffin, C. et al (2016)
Research: Alexithymia in children with and without autism spectrum disorders
Source: Autism Research. Vol. 9, pp. 773-780Author: Ola, L. and Gullon-Scott, F. (2020)
Research: Facial emotion recognition in autistic adult females correlates with alexithymia, not autism
Source: Autism. Vol. 24, pp. 2021-2034Author: Milosavljevic, B. et al (2016)
Research: Alexithymia in Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder: Its Relationship to Internalising Difficulties, Sensory Modulation and Social Cognition
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 46, pp. 1354-1367Author: Ryan, C. et al (2021)
Research: Objective and Subjective Measurement of Alexithymia in Adults with Autism
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 51, pp. 2019-2028Author: Gormley, E. et al (2021)
Research: Alexithymia is Associated with Emotion Dysregulation in Young People with Autism Spectrum Disorder
Source: Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities. Vol. OnlineFirst, pp. 1-16Author: Yi, H. and Siu, Q. (2021)
Research: “His inner-self must be good”: An ethnographic-vignette study of social interactions between children with autism spectrum disorder and typically developing peers
Source: Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. Vol. 38, pp. 232-255Author: Kelly, R. et al (2018)
Research: ‘Just trying to talk to people … It’s the hardest’: Perspectives of adolescents with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder on their social communication skills
Source: Child Language Teaching and Therapy. Vol. 34, pp. 319-334Author: Cummins, C. et al (2020)
Research: Autistic adults’ views of their communication skills and needs
Source: International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. Vol. 55, pp. 678-689Author: Nordahl-Hansen, A. et al (2016)
Research: Relations between specific and global outcome measures in a social-communication intervention for children with autism spectrum disorder
Source: Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Vol. 29, pp. 19-29.Author: Nordahl-Hansen, A. et al (2016)
Research: Relations between specific and global outcome measures in a social-communication intervention for children with autism spectrum disorder
Source: Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Vol. 29, pp. 19-29.Author: Qi, C. et al (2018)
Research: A Systematic Review of Single-Case Research Studies on Using Video Modeling Interventions to Improve Social Communication Skills for Individuals With Autism Spectrum Disorder
Source: Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities. Vol. 33, pp. 249-257Author: Dubin, A. and Lieberman-Betz, R. (2020)
Research: Naturalistic Interventions to Improve Prelinguistic Communication for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: a Systematic Review
Source: Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 7, pp. 151-167Author: Lerna, A. et al (2012)
Research: Social–communicative effects of the Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS) in Autism Spectrum Disorders
Source: International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. Vol. 47, pp. 609-617Author: Lerna, A. et al (2014)
Research: Long‐term effects of PECS on social–communicative skills of children with autism spectrum disorders: a follow‐up study
Source: International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. Vol. 49, pp. 478-485Author: Whitehouse et al (2021)
Research: Effect of Preemptive Intervention on Developmental Outcomes Among Infants Showing Early Signs of Autism. A Randomized Clinical Trial of Outcomes to Diagnosis
Source: JAMA Pediatrics, pp. 1-11Author: Birnschein, A. et al (2021)
Research: Enhancing social interactions for youth with autism spectrum disorder through training programs for typically developing peers: A systematic review
Source: Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Vol. 84, pp. 1-15Author: Watkins, L. et al (2017)
Research: A Meta-analysis of School-Based Social Interaction Interventions for Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder
Source: Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 4, pp. 277-293Author: Wilkes‐Gillan, S. and Falkmer, M. (2014)
Research: A peer‐mediated school intervention significantly improved the social skills and playground interactions of children with autism spectrum disorder
Source: Australian Occupational Therapy Journal. Vol. 61, pp. 371-374Author: Camargo, S. et al (2015)
Research: Behaviorally Based Interventions for Teaching Social Interaction Skills to Children with ASD in Inclusive Settings: A Meta-analysis
Source: Journal of Behavioral Education. Vol. 25, pp 223-248Author: Watkins, L. et al (2014)
Research: A Review of Peer-Mediated Social Interaction Interventions for Students with Autism in Inclusive Settings
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 45, pp. 1070-1083Author: Spain, D. et al (2017)
Research: Group cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT) for social interaction anxiety in adults with autism spectrum disorders (ASD)
Source: Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Vol. 41, pp. 20-30Author: Bauminger-Zviely, N. et al (2020)
Research: Preschool Peer Social Intervention (PPSI) to Enhance Social Play, Interaction, and Conversation: Study Outcomes
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 50, pp. 844-863Author: Lawton, K. and Kasari, C. (2012)
Research: Teacher-Implemented Joint Attention Intervention: Pilot Randomized Controlled Study for Preschoolers With Autism
Source: Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. Vol. 80, pp. 687-693Author: Hutchinson, N. and Bodicoat, A. (2015)
Research: The Effectiveness of Intensive Interaction, A Systematic Literature Review
Source: Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities. Vol. 28, pp. 437-454Author: Berridge, S. and Hutchinson, N. (2021)
Research: Staff experience of the implementation of intensive interaction within their places of work with people with learning disabilities and/or autism
Source: Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities. Vol. 34, pp. 1-15Author: Wood-Downie, H. et al (2021)
Research: Research Review: A systematic review and meta-analysis of sex/gender differences in social interaction and communication in autistic and non-autistic children and adolescents
Source: The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. Vol. 62, pp. 922-936Author: Van Wijngaarden-Cremers, P. et al (2014)
Research: Gender and Age Differences in the Core Triad of Impairments in Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis
Source: Sources: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 44, pp. 627-635Author: Matson, J. et al (2011)
Research: A multinational study examining the cross cultural differences in reported symptoms of autism spectrum disorders: Israel, South Korea, the United Kingdom, and the United States of America
Source: Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Vol. 5, pp. 1598-1604Author: Tek, S. and Landa, R. et al (2012)
Research: Differences in Autism Symptoms Between Minority and Non-Minority Toddlers
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 42, pp. 1967-1973Author: Harrison, A. et al (2017)
Research: Examining the Role of Race, Ethnicity, and Gender on Social and Behavioral Ratings Within the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 47, pp. 2770-2782Author: Levinson, S. et al (2020)
Research: Brief Report: Predicting Social Skills from Semantic, Syntactic, and Pragmatic Language Among Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 50, pp. 4165-4175Author: Friedman, L. et al (2019)
Research: Conversational Language Is a Predictor of Vocational Independence and Friendships in Adults with ASD
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 49, pp. 4294-4305Author: Rodas, N. et al (2017)
Research: Structural and Pragmatic Language in Children with ASD: Longitudinal Impact on Anxiety and Externalizing Behaviors
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 47, pp. 3479-3488Author: Chericoni, N. et al (2016)
Research: Pre-linguistic Vocal Trajectories at 6–18 Months of Age As Early Markers of Autism
Source: Frontiers in Psychology. Vol. 7, pp. 1-11Author: Patten, E. et al (2014)
Research: Vocal Patterns in Infants with Autism Spectrum Disorder: Canonical Babbling Status and Vocalization Frequency
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 44, pp. 2413-2428Author: Barone, R. et al (2019)
Research: Online comprehension across different semantic categories in preschool children with autism spectrum disorder
Source: PLoS ONE. Vol. 14, pp. 1-17Author: Davidson, M. and Ellis Weismer, S. (2017)
Research: A Discrepancy in Comprehension and Production in Early Language Development in ASD: Is it Clinically Relevant?
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 47, pp. 2163-2175Author: Suh, J. et al (2014)
Research: Narrative Performance of Optimal Outcome Children and Adolescents with a History of an Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD)
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 44, pp. 1681-1694Author: McCabe, A. et al (2013)
Research: Brief Report: Structure of Personal Narratives of Adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 43, pp. 733-738Author: Kenan, N. et al (2019)
Research: Semantic-Pragmatic Impairment in the Narratives of Children With Autism Spectrum Disorders
Source: Frontiers in Psychology. Vol. 10, pp. 1-13Author: Carlsson, E. et al (2020)
Research: Narrative Skills in Primary School Children with Autism in Relation to Language and Nonverbal Temporal Sequencing
Source: Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. Vol. 49, pp. 475-489Author: Nuske, H. and Bavin, E. (2011)
Research: Narrative comprehension in 4–7‐year‐old children with autism: testing the Weak Central Coherence account
Source: International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. Vol. 46, pp. 108-119Author: Kalandadze, T. et al (2016)
Research: Figurative language comprehension in individuals with autism spectrum disorder: A meta-analytic review
Source: Autism. Vol. 22, pp. 99-117Author: Chahboun, S. et al (2016)
Research: Can You Play with Fire and Not Hurt Yourself? A Comparative Study in Figurative Language Comprehension between Individuals with and without Autism Spectrum Disorder
Source: PLoS ONE. Vol. 11, pp. 1-24Author: Van Herwegen, J. and Rundblad, G. (2018)
Research: A Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Study of Novel Metaphor and Metonymy Comprehension in Children, Adolescents, and Adults With Autism Spectrum Disorder
Source: Frontiers in Psychology. Vol. 9, pp. 1-10Author: Kalandadze, T. et al (2021)
Research: Metaphor Comprehension in Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder: Core Language Skills Matter
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. OnlineFirst, pp. 1-11Author: Kjellmer, L. et al (2018)
Research: Speech and language profiles in 4- to 6-year-old children with early diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder without intellectual disability
Source: Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment. Vol. 14, pp. 2415-2427Author: Lazenby, D. et al (2016)
Research: Language Differences at 12 Months in Infants Who Develop Autism Spectrum Disorder
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 46, pp. 899-909Author: Barbaro, J. and Dissanayake, C. (2012)
Research: Developmental profiles of infants and toddlers with autism spectrum disorders identified prospectively in a community-based setting
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 42, pp. 1939-1948Author: Seol, K. et al (2014)
Research: A comparison of receptive-expressive language profiles between toddlers with autism spectrum disorder and developmental language delay.
Source: Yonsei Medical Journal, Vol. 55, pp. 1721-1728.Author: Rose, V. et al (2020)
Research: Predictors of Expressive Language Change for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Receiving AAC-Infused Comprehensive Intervention
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 50, pp. 278-291Author: White, E. et al (2021)
Research: Augmentative and Alternative Communication and Speech Production for Individuals with ASD: A Systematic Review
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. OnlineFirst, pp. 1-14Author: Schreibman, L. and Stahmer, A. (2014)
Research: A Randomized Trial Comparison of the Effects of Verbal and Pictorial Naturalistic Communication Strategies on Spoken Language for Young Children with Autism
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 44, pp. 1244-1251Author: Van Santen, J. (2013)
Research: Quantifying repetitive speech in autism spectrum disorders and language impairment
Source: Autism Research. Vol. 6, pp. 372-383Author: Norbury, C. and Bishop, D. (2003)
Research: Narrative skills of children with communication impairments
Source: International journal of language and communication disorders. Vol. 38, pp. 287-313.Author: Lake, J. et al, (2011)
Research: Listener vs. speaker-oriented aspects of speech: Studying the disfluencies of individuals with autism spectrum disorders
Source: Psychonomic bulletin and review. Vol. 18, pp. 135-140.Author: Strauss, K. et al (2012)
Research: Parent inclusion in early intensive behavioral intervention: The influence of parental stress, parent treatment fidelity and parent-mediated generalization of behavior targets on child outcomes
Source: Research in developmental disabilities. Vol. 33, pp. 688-703.Author: Reichow et al (2012)
Research: Overview of meta-analyses on early intensive behavioral intervention for young children with autism spectrum disorders
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 42, pp. 512-520.Author: Barger, B. et al (2013)
Research: Prevalence and onset of regression within autism spectrum disorders: a meta-analytic review.
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 43, pp. 817-828.Author: Gagnon, D. (2021)
Research: Bayonet-shaped language development in autism with regression: a retrospective study
Source: Molecular Autism. Vol. 12, pp. 1-12Author: Hull et al (2017)
Research: ‘Putting on My Best Normal’; Social Camouflaging in Adults with Autism Spectrum Conditions
Source: Journal of autism and developmental disorders. Vol. 47, pp. 2519-2534.Author: Halsall et al (2021)
Research: ‘Camouflaging’ by adolescent autistic girls who attend both mainstream and specialist resources classes: perspectives of girls, their mothers and their educators
Source: Autism. Vol. 25, pp. 2074-2086Author: Sturrock et al (2019a)
Research: Observational and reported measures of language and pragmatics in young people with autism: A comparison of respondent data and gender profiles
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 50, pp. 812-830Author: Sturrock et al (2019b)
Research: Speaking the same language? A preliminary investigation, comparing the language and communication skills of females and males with high-functioning autism
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 50, pp. 1639-1656Author: Parish-Morris et al (2017)
Research: Linguistic camouflage in girls with autism spectrum disorder
Source: Molecular Autism. Vol. 8, pp. 1-12Author: Kauschke et al (2016)
Research: Narratives of Girls and Boys with Autism Spectrum Disorders: Gender Differences in Narrative Competence and Internal State Language
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 46, pp. 840-852Author: Sturrock et al (2021)
Research: In their own words: The impact of subtle language and communication difficulties as described by autistic girls and boys without intellectual disability
Source: Autism. Vol. 26, pp. 332-345Author: Brewer et al (2015)
Research: Can Neurotypical Individuals Read Autistic Facial Expressions? Atypical Production of Emotional Facial Expressions in Autism Spectrum Disorders
Source: Autism Research. Vol. 9, pp. 262-271Author: Fan et al (2013)
Research: Empathic arousal and social understanding in individuals with autism: evidence from fMRI and ERP measurement
Source: Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. Vol. 9, pp. 1203-1213Author: Rieffe et al (2020)
Research: Quantity and quality of empathic responding by autistic and non-autistic adolescent girls and boys
Source: Autism. Vol. 25, pp. 199-209Author: Asberg (2010)
Research: Patterns of language and discourse comprehension skills in school-aged children with autism spectrum disorders
Source: Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. Vol. 51, pp. 534-539Author: Boorse et al (2019)
Research: Linguistic markers of autism in girls: evidence of a “blended phenotype” during storytelling
Source: Molecular Autism. Vol. 10, pp. 1-12Author: Siller et al (2014)
Research: Internal state language in the storybook narratives of children with and without autism spectrum disorder: Investigating relations to theory of mind abilities
Source: Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Vol. 8, pp. 589-596Author: Stiegler (2015)
Research: Examining the Echolalia Literature: Where Do Speech-Language Pathologists Stand?
Source: American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. Vol. 24, pp. 750-762.Author: Underhill et al (2019)
Research: Autism stigma in communication classrooms: exploring peer attitudes and motivations toward interacting with atypical students.
Source: Communication Education. Vol. 68, pp. 175-192Author: Botha et al (2020)
Research: "Autism is me": an investigation of how autistic individuals make sense of autism and stigma
Source: Disability and Society. Pp. 1-27Author: Perry et al (2022)
Research: Understanding camouflaging as a response to autism-related stigma: A Social Identity Theory approach
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 52, pp. 800-810Author: Cage et al (2018)
Research: Experiences of Autism Acceptance and Mental Health in Autistic Adults
Source: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Vol. 48, pp. 473-484Author: Williams et al (2021)
Research: Mutual (Mis)understanding: Reframing Autistic Pragmatic “Impairments” Using Relevance Theory
Source: Frontiers in Psychology. Vol. 12, pp. 1-21Author: Howard and Sedgewick (2021)
Research: ‘Anything but the phone!’: Communication mode preferences in the autism community
Source: Autism. Vol. 25, pp. 2265-2278Author: Dean and Chang (2021)
Research: A Systematic Review of School-Based Social Skills Interventions and Observed Social Outcomes for Students with Autism Spectrum Disorder in Inclusive Settings
Source: Autism. Vol. 25, pp. 1828-1843Author: Loukusa and Moilanen (2009)
Research: Pragmatic inference abilities in individuals with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. A review
Source: Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders.Author: Jaswal et al (2018)
Research: Being versus appearing socially uninterested: Challenging assumptions about social motivation in autism
Source: Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 42, E82. doi:10.1017/S0140525X18001826Professional resources
Author: NICE (2017)
Research: Autism spectrum disorder in under 19s: recognition, referral and diagnosis
Source: Clinical guideline [CG128], Published: 28 September 2011 Last updated: 20 December 2017Author: NICE (2017)
Research: Autism spectrum disorder in under 19s: recognition, referral and diagnosis
Source: Clinical guideline [CG128]Author: NICE (2017)
Research: Autism spectrum disorder in under 19s: recognition, referral and diagnosis
Source: Clinical guideline [CG128]Published: 28 September 2011 Last updated: 20 December 2017Author: NICE (2021)
Research: Interventions for core features of autism spectrum disorder
Source: NICEAuthor: NICE (2021)
Research: Psychosocial interventions for core symptoms
Source: NICEAuthor: Ambitious About Autism (accessed August 2021)
Research: What is Autism?
Source: Ambitious About AutismAuthor: Cambridgeshire Community Services; Accessed August 2021) NHS Trust
Research: Social Communication Difficulties and Autistic Spectrum Conditions: Community Paediatrics
Source: NHS CambridgeshireAuthor: Autism Tasmania (accessed August 2021)
Research: About Autism: Social Communication
Source: Autism TasmaniaAuthor: RCSLT (accessed August 2021)
Research: Social Communication Disorder- overview
Source: RCSLTAuthor: Hanen Centre (accessed August 2021)
Research: How Autism Affects Communication in Young Children
Source: Hanen CentreAuthor: American Speech Language Hearing Association (accessed August 2021)
Research: Social Communication Disorder
Source: American Speech Language Hearing AssociationAuthor: Autism Early Support Circle Centre (accessed August 2021)
Research: Speech and language Therapy
Source: Autism Early Support Circle CentreAuthor: The Hanen Centre (2012)
Research: Communication-Building Tips
Source: for Parents of Children with Autism Spectrum DisorderAuthor: The Hanen Centre (accessed September 2021)
Research: Encouraging Pretend Play in Children with Social Communication Difficulties
Source: The Hanen CentreAuthor: The Hanen Centre (Accessed September 2021)
Research: R.O.C.K.™ in People Games: Building Communication in Children with ASD or Social Communication Difficulties
Source: HanenAuthor: Hanen Centre (accessed September 2021)
Research: Welcome to Autism Corner- Build Your Child’s Communication Skills – All Day Long
Source: HanenAuthor: NICE (2011)
Research: 1.5 Autism diagnostic assessment for children and young people
Source: NICEAuthor: NICE (2021)
Research: Autism Research Recommendations
Source: NICEAuthor: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association (Accessed September 2021)
Research: Echolalia and Its Role in Gestalt Language Acquisition
Source: American Speech-Language-Hearing AssociationAuthor: The Hanen Centre (Accessed September 2021)
Research: Communication Development in Children with Autism
Source: The Hanen CentreAuthor: NAS (Accessed January 2022)
Research: DISCO
Source: Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders frameworkAuthor: The International Society for Augmentative and Alternative Communication
Research: Homepage
Source: Website (charity)Author: The Royal College of Speech and Language Therapy
Research: Augmentative and alternative communication (AAC) overview
Source: WebsiteAuthor: Jane Farrall (2015)
Research: What is “Beginning AAC”?
Source: BlogAuthor: Communication Matters (accessed May 2022)
Research: What is AAC?
Source: ArticleAuthor: Caldwell (accessed May 2022
Research: Intensive Interaction
Source: Phoebe Caldwell websiteAuthor: Play Connect (May 2020)
Research: Echolalia
Source: Play ConnectLived experience
Author: Alex Lowery (2021a)
Research: Struggling to act ‘cool’ and silly as a teen with autism
Source: BlogAuthor: Alex Lowery (2017)
Research: Are people with Aspergers/autism chatterboxes or super quiet?
Source: BlogAuthor: Alex Lowery (2016)
Research: Are people with autism unsociable?
Source: BlogAuthor: Alex Lowery (2015a)
Research: Autism and social media
Source: BlogAuthor: Alex Lowery (2015b)
Research: Why are social rules complicated for a person with autism?
Source: BlogAuthor: Sylvia Lowery and Alex Lowery (2015)
Research: Should we teach children with autism to play?
Source: BlogAuthor: Alex Lowery (2017)
Research: Nonverbal communication from an autistic’s perspective
Source: BlogAuthor: Jamie Knight (2014)
Research: Speech
Source: BlogAuthor: Jamie Knight (2017)
Research: No masking
Source: BlogAuthor: Jamie Knight (2016a)
Research: My Speech
Source: BlogAuthor: Jamie Knight (2016b)
Research: Week 53 - Pacing & Fear of Missing Out.
Source: BlogAuthor: Gavin Bollard (2013)
Research: Friendship isn’t so obvious when you’re on the spectrum
Source: BlogAuthor: Gavin Bollard (2014)
Research: Why is Empathy so hard for people with Asperger's Syndrome?
Source: BlogAuthor: Amy Sequenzia (2012)
Research: Interview: Amy Sequenzia on Facilitated Communication
Source: Interview/blogAuthor: Helen Needham (2020)
Research: Neurodiversity Tips: Avoiding social overload
Source: BlogAuthor: Neurodivergent Thinkers (2018)
Research: The Problems with talking - musings of an autistic mind
Source: BlogAuthor: Lisa Ventura (2018)
Research: What the World Doesn't See - #TakeTheMaskOff
Source: BlogAuthor: Helen Needham (2018)
Research: Unmasked #TakeTheMaskOff - lifting the lid on a life spent hiding
Source: BlogAuthor: Paul Isaacs (2014)
Research: All Communication Is Valid – None Are Inferior
Source: BlogAuthor: One Lone Dandelion (2018)
Research: Verbal and Written Communication: The Disconnect
Source: BlogAuthor: Adam Lodestone (2021)
Research: “That’s SO RUDE!” Is It, Though? (Helping To Bridge The Gap Between NT & ND Communication Needs)
Source: BlogAuthor: Jorn Bettin (2019)
Research: What CAN be misunderstood WILL be misunderstood
Source: BlogAuthor: Olympia Ellinas (2019)
Research: Why AAC is a Human Right for NonSpeaking Autistics and Disabled People
Source: BlogAuthor: The Autistic Advocate (2017)
Research: Talking without words: Communication, Autism and how I needed someone to tell me, that It’s OK to be me
Source: BlogAuthor: Musings of an Aspie (2014a)
Research: Echolalia and scripting: straddling the border of functional language
Source: BlogAuthor: Musings of an Aspie (2014b)
Research: Prosody: Loud voice, fast voice, soft voice, flat voice
Source: BlogAuthor: Musings of an Aspie (2013a)
Research: (Not) A Little Slow
Source: BlogAuthor: Musings of an Aspie (2013b)
Research: The Empathy Conundrum
Source: BlogAuthor: Musings of an Aspie (2013c)
Research: Emotional dysfunction: Alexithymia and ASD
Source: BlogAuthor: Steve Slavin (2015)
Research: The Mystery of Social Interaction
Source: BlogAuthor: Steve Slavin (2013a)
Research: Autism: “auditory dyslexia”
Source: BlogAuthor: Steve Slavin (2013b)
Research: Autism: Successful communication, 10 things to make it easier
Source: BlogAuthor: Rosie Weldon (2021)
Research: Autism and emotions
Source: BlogAuthor: Quincy (2019a)
Research: Asking direct questions
Source: BlogAuthor: Quincy (2019b)
Research: Different social skills, not a lack thereof.
Source: BlogAuthor: Max Sparrow (2017)
Research: Alexithymia: I Don’t Know How I Feel
Source: BlogAuthor: Kate Swenson (2020)
Research: Ten Years Old and Nonverbal
Source: BlogAuthor: The Autism Page (accessed 05/11/2021)
Research: The importance of communication – a dad’s view
Source: BlogAuthor: Living with a Jude (2019)
Research: Intensive Interaction
Source: BlogAuthor: Miriam Gwynne (2019)
Research: Three Things I Have Learnt About Children’s Speech From My Non Verbal Son
Source: BlogAuthor: Neurodivergent Rebel (2018a)
Research: An Autistic Perspective - Communication Styles
Source: YouTubeAuthor: Neurodivergent Rebel (2021)
Research: I'm Autistic and this is Why I Find People Overwhelming
Source: YouTubeAuthor: Purple Ella (2017a)
Research: AUTISM AND TALKING ON THE PHONE| Purple Ella
Source: YouTubeAuthor: Purple Ella (2017b)
Research: AUTISM AND MASKING |Purple Ella
Source: YouTubeAuthor: The Aspie World (2020)
Research: Autism Communication | THIS Is How YOU communicate with Autistic People (5 TIPS)
Source: YouTubeAuthor: The Aspie World (2021)
Research: AUTISM Faces - [Why YOU Don’t Show Emotion]
Source: YouTubeAuthor: The Aspie World (2018)
Research: AUTISM eye contact: MUST know info
Source: YouTubeAuthor: IndieAndy (2019)
Research: How Can Social Media Help Autistic People?
Source: YouTubeAuthor: IndieAndy (2019)
Research: Can Social Media be Dangerous for Autistic people?
Source: YouTubeAuthor: Yo Samdy Sam (2019)
Research: Why autistic people seem two-faced
Source: YouTubeAuthor: Autmazing (2020)
Research: Autism and Eye Contact- One Autistic Adults Experience
Source: YouTubeAuthor: Matthew Weatherford (2021)
Research: What It's Like Having Hyperempathy as an Autistic Person
Source: BlogAuthor: David Hopkins (2021)
Research: Why I’m not just a little bit autistic: the wonders of neurodiversity
Source: BlogAuthor: Purple Ella (2021)
Research: Friends When You're Autistic (A Chat With My Neurodivergent Friends)
Source: YouTubeAuthor: Terra Vance (2021)
Research: Weavers and Concluders: Two Communication Styles No One Knows Exist
Source: BlogAuthor: Trauma Geek (2020)
Research: Lost in Translation: The Social Language Theory of Neurodivergence
Source: BlogAuthor: Endever* (2019)
Research: We deserve access to AAC!
Source: Blog